来月经小腹痛是什么原因| 为什么老打嗝| 深夜里买醉是什么歌| 纯爱是什么意思| 柯是什么意思| 大便拉不干净是什么原因| 芝士是什么| 少白头是什么原因| 长命锁一般由什么人送| 食管息肉的症状是什么| 切勿是什么意思| 秋千为什么叫秋千| 三纲指的是什么| 下午1点到3点是什么时辰| 家里为什么会有蜈蚣| 查转氨酶挂什么科| 超纤皮是什么| hrv是什么| 梦到涨大水预示着什么| 黄金果是什么水果| 7777什么意思| 吹弹可破的意思是什么| 黑脸娃娃有什么功效| 宫腔镜检查主要查什么| 腿麻是什么原因| 杜鹃花什么颜色| 后羿代表什么生肖| dwi是什么检查| 高血压是什么引起的| lol锤石什么时候出的| 幽门螺旋杆菌吃什么药| 为什么手术服是绿色的| 春天穿什么衣服| 什么奶粉跟母乳一个味| 体重什么时候称最准确| 功名是什么意思| fov是什么| 肌电图是检查什么的| 什么品牌的奶粉最好| 蒸鱼豉油可以用什么代替| 实蛋是什么| 丿是什么字| 未时是什么时辰| 为什么会得甲减| 过敏性鼻炎不能吃什么| 面色少华是什么意思| 手心有痣代表什么意思| 容忍是什么意思| 302是什么意思| 阴囊瘙痒是什么原因| 小孩子睡觉磨牙是什么原因| 一什么菜地| homme是什么意思| 雌二醇高说明什么原因| 什么东西能美白| 乙肝看什么科| 出生证编号是什么| 小精灵是什么意思| 腿抽筋用什么药| 同学过生日送什么礼物好| 白细胞2个加号是什么意思| 出痧是什么原因| 眉毛长长是什么原因| 炭疽是什么| 焦虑症是什么原因引起的| 蚂蚁最怕什么| 什么时候艾灸最好| 为什么越累越胖| ct平扫能检查出什么| 燊念什么| 牛仔外套搭配什么裤子好看| xpe是什么材料| 说话鼻音重是什么原因| 腋窝淋巴结肿大挂什么科| 白蛋白高是什么原因| 菟丝子有什么功效| 幽门杆菌是什么意思| 嘴巴苦是什么原因| 真丝棉是什么面料| 女人下面 什么味道| fw是什么意思| 扶乩是什么意思| 珑字五行属什么| 830是什么意思| hg是什么意思| 五个月宝宝吃什么辅食最好| 将军是指什么生肖| 三高不能吃什么食物| 屋漏偏逢连夜雨是什么意思| 致字五行属什么| 更年期什么意思| 梦见老公穿新衣服是什么意思| 拉稀是什么原因| 宝宝拉肚子吃什么药好得快| 川军为什么那么出名| 胰岛素是干什么用的| 7月份可以种什么菜| 莆田医院是什么意思| 绿松石是什么| 螃蟹吃什么东西| 骨加后念什么| 经常爱放屁是什么原因| ppsu是什么材质| 生化全项包括什么| 后年是什么年| 犹太是什么意思| 无常是什么意思| 浣碧什么时候背叛甄嬛| 周杰伦有什么病| 梦见吵架是什么意思| 白天不懂夜的黑什么意思| PSV是什么意思| 大腿根部内侧瘙痒用什么药膏| 吃什么可以降低尿酸| 犀利是什么意思| 玉米什么时候传入中国| 包可以加什么偏旁| 孙尚香字什么| 肠胀气是什么原因引起的怎么解决| 李五行属什么| 夏天不出汗是什么原因| 牛排和什么一起炖好吃| cathy是什么意思| 为什么会得带状疱疹| 兔死狐悲指什么生肖| 春秋鼎盛是什么意思| mop是什么意思| 刘姥姥进大观园什么意思| 吃什么血脂降的最快| 少了一个肾有什么影响| 3月27日什么星座| 猫可以吃什么水果| 乙状结肠腺瘤是什么病| 今年77岁属什么生肖| 好好活着比什么都重要| 陌上花开可缓缓归矣什么意思| 足底筋膜炎什么症状| 皮蛋与什么食物相克| hpv检查是什么| 什么人容易高反| 右手麻木是什么原因引起的| 扦脚是什么意思| 楚楚动人是什么意思| 呵呵的含义是什么意思| 睡醒嘴苦是什么原因| 犹太人说什么语言| 痰的颜色代表什么| 点了斑不能吃什么| 眼压是什么| 酉时是什么时候| 血氧仪是干什么用的| 左眼上眼皮跳是什么预兆| 1129什么星座| mar什么意思| 糖尿病人可以吃什么水果| 丝瓜为什么会苦| 白羊座后面是什么星座| 为什么医生爱开喜炎平| 吃什么容易结石| 白蛋白偏低是什么原因| 教育的本质是什么| pacu是什么意思| 减肥什么方法有效| 杭州吃什么| 物流专员是做什么的| 12月18日什么星座| 精子成活率低吃什么药| 一劳永逸什么意思| 老是瞌睡是什么原因| 什么叫跨境电商| 窦性心律t波改变是什么意思| 深圳有什么好吃的| 松塔有什么用| 高血糖吃什么菜好| 欣是什么意思| 肝结节是什么意思| 肾虚吃什么补最好| 女生什么时候最容易怀孕| 12月2号什么星座| 牙齿冷热都疼是什么原因| 1990年是什么年| 鱼不能和什么食物一起吃| 为什么手脚冰凉还出汗| 4月11号是什么星座| 反物质是什么| 韭菜什么时候种最合适| 每天流鼻血是什么原因| 双响炮是什么| 早搏是什么意思| 建字五行属什么| 饮食清淡的标准是什么| 卧轨什么意思| 鱼刺卡喉咙去医院挂什么科| 什么是白平衡| 快递客服主要做什么| 哮喘挂什么科| 上户口需要什么资料| 圆房是什么意思| 海字五行属什么| 户主有什么权利| 菩提子长什么样| 婴儿什么时候可以睡枕头| 香肠炒什么菜好吃| 头孢是什么| 四风指什么| 怀孕喝什么牛奶好| 牛肉饺子馅配什么蔬菜好吃| 42年属什么生肖| 天团是什么意思| cmyk代表什么颜色| 慢性萎缩性胃炎伴糜烂吃什么药| 梦见杀人了是什么意思| 神经电生理检查是什么| 痛风是什么病| warning什么意思| hyundai是什么牌子| 朗字五行属什么| 送孕妇什么礼物最贴心| 大祭司是什么意思| 桃胶什么时候采摘最好| 什么是食物链| 什么的天空填词语| 肝郁症是什么病| 什么是规律| 现在什么年| 晕车吃什么药| 勇者胜的上半句是什么| 藏青色t恤配什么颜色裤子| 什么人不能吃蚕豆| 破伤风是什么| 驳是什么意思| 为什么来我家| 五味杂陈什么意思| 食物中毒拉肚子吃什么药| 乌龟不吃食是什么原因| 手上长老年斑是什么原因| 什么人容易得心梗| 宜宾燃面为什么叫燃面| 为什么要喝酒| 藏族信仰什么教| 做完核磁共振后需要注意什么| 整天放屁是什么原因| 扁平足是什么样的| 心慌是什么原因引起的| 黑枸杞泡水喝有什么好处| 手指长痣代表什么| 大腿内侧什么经络| 256排ct能检查什么病| 拉肚子吃什么食物| 抗炎是什么意思| 眼睛干涩模糊用什么药| 什么伤口需要打破伤风| 此物非彼物是什么意思| 关税是什么意思| 腿上无缘无故出现淤青是什么原因| 仓鼠咬笼子是什么原因| 蚯蚓可以钓什么鱼| 尿路感染吃什么药好得快| 什么是原发性高血压| 男人吃秋葵有什么好处| 什么动物是站着睡觉的| 6月13是什么星座| 没什么大不了| 冷血动物是什么意思| 百度

专家研讨电视剧《信访局长》

(Redirected from Phonemic split)
百度 这是我们党经受住执政考验的道义支撑和根本价值取向。

In historical linguistics, phonological change is any sound change that alters the distribution of phonemes in a language. In other words, a language develops a new system of oppositions among its phonemes. Old contrasts may disappear, new ones may emerge, or they may simply be rearranged.[1] Sound change may be an impetus for changes in the phonological structures of a language (and likewise, phonological change may sway the process of sound change).[1] One process of phonological change is rephonemicization, in which the distribution of phonemes changes by either addition of new phonemes or a reorganization of existing phonemes.[2] Mergers and splits are types of rephonemicization and are discussed further below.

Types

edit

In a typological scheme first systematized by Henry M. Hoenigswald in 1965, a historical sound law can only affect a phonological system in one of three ways:

  • Conditioned merger (which Hoenigswald calls "primary split"), in which some instances of phoneme A become an existing phoneme B; the number of phonemes does not change, only their distribution.
  • Phonemic split (which Hoenigswald calls "secondary split"), in which some instances of A become a new phoneme B; this is phonemic differentiation in which the number of phonemes increases.
  • Unconditioned merger, in which all instances of phonemes A and B become A; this is phonemic reduction, in which the number of phonemes decreases.

This classification does not consider mere changes in pronunciation, that is, phonetic change, even chain shifts, in which neither the number nor the distribution of phonemes is affected.

Phonetic vs. phonological change

edit

Phonetic change can occur without any modification to the phoneme inventory or phonemic correspondences. This change is purely allophonic or subphonemic. This can entail one of two changes: either the phoneme turns into a new allophone—meaning the phonetic form changes—or the distribution of allophones of the phoneme changes.[2]

For the most part, phonetic changes are examples of allophonic differentiation or assimilation; i.e., sounds in specific environments acquire new phonetic features or perhaps lose phonetic features they originally had. For example, the devoicing of the vowels /i/ and /?/ in certain environments in Japanese, the nasalization of vowels before nasals (common but not universal), changes in point of articulation of stops and nasals under the influence of adjacent vowels.

Phonetic change in this context refers to the lack of phonological restructuring, not a small degree of sound change. For example, chain shifts such as the Great Vowel Shift (in which nearly all of the vowels of the English language changed) or the allophonic differentiation of /s/, originally *[s], into [s z ? ? ? ? θ χ χ? h], do not qualify as phonological change as long as all of the phones remain in complementary distribution.

Many phonetic changes provide the raw ingredients for later phonemic innovations. In Proto-Italic, for example, intervocalic */s/ became *[z]. It was a phonetic change, merely a mild and superficial complication in the phonological system, but when *[z] merged with */r/, the effect on the phonological system was greater. (The example will be discussed below, under conditioned merger.)

Similarly, in the prehistory of Indo-Iranian, the velars */k/ and */g/ acquired distinctively palatal articulation before front vowels (*/e/, */i/, */ē/ */ī/), so that */ke/ came to be pronounced *[t??e] and */ge/ *[d??e], but the phones *[t??] and *[d??] occurred only in that environment. However, when */e/, */o/, */a/ later fell together as Proto-Indo-Iranian */a/ (and */ē/ */ō/ */ā/ likewise fell together as */ā/), the result was that the allophonic palatal and velar stops now contrasted in identical environments: */ka/ and /?a/, /ga/ and /?a/, and so on. The difference became phonemic. (The "law of palatals" is an example of phonemic split.)

Sound changes generally operate for a limited period of time, and once established, new phonemic contrasts rarely remain tied to their ancestral environments. For example, Sanskrit acquired "new" /ki/ and /gi/ sequences via analogy and borrowing, and likewise /?u/, /?u/, /?m/, and similar novelties; and the reduction of the diphthong */ay/ to Sanskrit /ē/ had no effect at all on preceding velar stops.

Merger

edit

Phonemic merger is a loss of distinction between phonemes. Occasionally, the term reduction refers to phonemic merger. It is not to be confused with the meaning of the word "reduction" in phonetics, such as vowel reduction, but phonetic changes may contribute to phonemic mergers. For example, in most North American English dialects, the vowel in the word lot and vowel in the word palm have become the same sound and thus undergone a merger. In most dialects of England, the words father and farther are pronounced the same due to a merger created by non-rhoticity or "R-dropping".

Conditioned merger

edit

Conditioned merger, or primary split, takes place when some, but not all, allophones of a phoneme, say A, merge with some other phoneme, B. The immediate results are these:

  • there are the same number of contrasts as before.
  • there are fewer words with A than before.
  • there are more words of B than before.
  • there is at least one environment for which A, for the time being, no longer occurs, called a gap in the distribution of the phoneme.
  • there is, under certain circumstances, an alternation between A and B if inflection or derivation result in A sometimes but not always being in the environment in which it merged with B.

Example from Middle English

edit

For a simple example, without alternation, early Middle English /d/ after stressed syllables followed by /r/ became /e/: módor, f?der > mother, father /er/, weder > weather, and so on. Since /e/ was already a structure-point in the language, the innovation resulted merely in more /e/ and less /d/ and a gap in the distribution of /d/ (though not a very conspicuous one).

Note 1: thanks to borrowing, from dialects as well as other languages, the original distribution has been disturbed: rudder, adder in Standard English (but forms with /e/ are attested in nonstandard dialects).
Note 2: one who knows German can figure out which cases of English /e/ were originally /e/ and which changed from /d/. Original /d/ corresponds to /t/ in German, and original /e/ corresponds to /d/. Thus, leather = German Leder, brother = Bruder, whether = weder, wether = Widder, pointing to original /e/ in English; weather = German Wetter, father = Vater, mother = Mutter pointing to original /d/.
Note 3: alternation between /d/ and /e/ would have been a theoretical possibility in English, as in sets like hard, harder; ride, rider, but any such details have been erased by the commonplace diachronic process called morphological leveling.

Devoicing of voiced stops in German

edit

A trivial (if all-pervasive) example of conditioned merger is the devoicing of voiced stops in German when in word-final position or immediately before a compound boundary (see: Help:IPA/Standard German):

  • *hand "hand" > /hant/ (cf. plural H?nde /?h?nd?/)
  • Handgelenk "wrist" /?hantg?l??k/
  • *bund "league, association" > /b?nt/ (cf. plural Bünde /?b?nd?/)
  • *gold "gold" > /g?lt/
  • *halb "half" > /halp/ (cf. halbieren "to halve" /hal?bi???n/)
  • halbamtlich "semi-official" /?halp?amtl??/
  • *berg "mountain" /b???k/ (cf. plural Berge /?b???g?/)
  • *klug "clever, wise" > /klu?k/ (cf. fem. kluge /?klu?g?/)

There were, of course, also many cases of original voiceless stops in final position: Bett "bed", bunt "colorful", Stock "(walking) stick, cane". To sum up: there are the same number of structure points as before, /p t k b d g/, but there are more cases of /p t k/ than before and fewer of /b d g/, and there is a gap in the distribution of /b d g/ (they are never found in word-final position or before a compound boundary).

Note 1: this split is easily recoverable by internal reconstruction because it results in alternations whose conditions are transparent. Thus Bund "bunch" (as in, keys) /b?nt/ has a plural Bünde /?b?nd?/ in contrast to bunt "colorful" with /t/ in all environments (feminine /?b?nt?/, neuter /?b?nt?s/ and so on). In a neutralizing environment, such as a voiceless stop in word-final position, one cannot tell which of two possibilities was the original sound. The choice is resolved if the corresponding segment can be found in a non-neutralizing position, as when a suffix follows. Accordingly, a non-inflected form like und /??nt/ "and" is historically opaque (though as the spelling hints, the /t/ was originally *d).
Note 2: unlike most phonological changes, this one became a "surface" rule in German, so loan-words whose source had a voiced stop in the devoicing environment are taken into German with a voiceless one instead: Klub "club" (association) /kl?p/ from English club. The same goes for truncated forms: Bub (for formal Bube "boy") is /bu?p/.
Note 2a: the surface alternation is what allows modern German orthography to write stops morphophonemically, thus Leib "loaf", Hand "hand", Weg "way", all with voiceless final stops in the simplex form and in compounds, but /b d g/ in inflected forms. In Old High and Middle High German, all voiceless stops were written as pronounced: hleip, hant, uuec and so on.
Note 3: the same distribution holds for /s/ vs. /z/, but it arose by a completely different process, the voicing of original */s/ between vowels: *mūs "mouse" > Maus /ma?s/, *mūsīz "mice" (for earlier *mūsiz) > M?use /?m??z?/. Original long (now short) ss does not voice medially, as in küssen "to kiss" /?k?sen/, nor does /s/ from Proto-West-Germanic *t, as in Wasser "water" /?vas?/, F?sser "kegs" /?f?s?/ plural of Fass /fas/ (= English vat), mü?ig "idle" /?my?s??/. German /?/, as in Fisch "fish", reflects original *sk (in native words) and does not become voiced in any environment: Fischer "fisherman" /?f???/. (German has /?/ only in loanwords: Genie /?e?ni?/ "genius", Gage /?ɡa???/ "salary".)

Rhotacism in Latin

edit

More typical of the aftermath of a conditioned merger is the famous case of rhotacism in Latin (also seen in some Sabellian language spoken in the same area): Proto-Italic *s > Latin /r/ between vowels: *gesō "I do, act" > Lat. gerō (but perfect gessi < *ges-s- and participle gestus < *ges-to-, etc., with unchanged *s in all other environments, even in the same paradigm).

This sound law is quite complete and regular, and in its immediate wake there were no examples of /s/ between vowels except for a few words with a special condition (miser "wretched", caesariēs "bushy hair", diser(c)tus "eloquent": that is, rhotacism did not take place when an /r/ followed the *s). However, a new crop of /s/ between vowels soon arose from three sources. (1) a shortening of /ss/ after a diphthong or long vowel: causa "lawsuit" < *kawssā, cāsa "house' < *kāssā, fūsus "poured, melted" < *χewssos. (2) univerbation: nisi (nisī) "unless" < the phrase *ne sei, quasi (quasī) "as if" < the phrase *k?am sei. (3) borrowings, such as rosa "rose" /rosa/, from a Sabellian source (the word is clearly somehow from Proto-Italic *ruθ- "red" but equally clearly not native Latin), and many words taken from or through Greek (philosophia, basis, casia, Mesopotamia, etc., etc.).

Nasal assimilation and "gn" in Latin

edit

A particular example of a conditioned merger in Latin is the rule whereby syllable-final stops, when followed by a nasal consonant, assimilated with it in nasality, while preserving their original point of articulation:

  • *supimos > *supmos > summus "highest"
  • *sabnyom > Samnium "Samnium" (a region in the southern Apennines)
  • *swepnos > somnus "sleep"
  • *atnos > annus "year"

In some cases, the underlying (pre-assimilation) root can be retrieved from related lexical items in the language: e.g. superior "higher"; Sabīni "Samnites"; sopor "(deep) sleep". For some words, only comparative evidence can help retrieve the original consonant: for example, the etymology of annus "year" (as *atnos) is revealed by comparison with Gothic atna "year".

According to this rule of nasal assimilation, the sequences *-g-n and *-k-n would become [?n], with a velar nasal [?]:

  • *dek-no- > dignus [di?nus] "worthy"
  • *leg-no- (*le?- "gather") > lignum [li?num] "firewood"
  • *teg-no- (*(s)teg- "build") > tignum [ti?num] "timber"
  • *ag?nos > *ag-nos[3] > agnus [a?nus] "lamb"

The sound [?] was not a phoneme of Latin, but an allophone of /g/ before /n/.

The sequence [?n] was regularly rendered in the orthography as |gn|.[4] Some epigraphic inscriptions also feature non-standard spellings, e.g. SINNU for signum "sign, insigne", INGNEM for ignem "fire". These are witness to the speakers' hesitancy on how to best transcribe the sound [?] in the sequence [?n].

The regular nasal assimilation of Latin can be seen as a form of "merger", insofar as it resulted in the contrast between oral stops (p, b, t, d) and nasal stops (m, n) being regularly neutralized.

Concerning the number of contrasts

edit

One of the traits of conditioned merger, as outlined above, is that the total number of contrasts remains the same, but it is possible for such splits to reduce the number of contrasts. It happens if all of the conditioned merger products merge with one or another phoneme.

For example, in Latin, the Pre-Latin phoneme *θ (from Proto-Italic *t? < PIE *dh) disappears as such by merging with three other sounds: *f (from PIE *bh and *g?h), *d, and *b:

Initially *θ > f:

  • PItal. *t?i-n-k?- "model, shape" > *θi-n-χ- > Lat. fingō (PIE root *dhey?h- "smear, work with the hands"; cf. Sanskrit dihanti "they smear", Avestan daēza- "wall" = Greek te?khos; English dough < OE dāh besides dāg < PIE *dhoy?h-)
  • PItal. *t?wor- "door" > *θwor- > Lat. forēs "door" (PIE *dhwor-; like most reflexes plural only; cf Eng. door < *dhur-, Greek thúrā (probably < *dhwor-) usually thúrai pl.)
Cf. Latin ferō "carry" < Proto-Italic *p?er- < PIE *bher-; Latin frāter "brother" < Proto-Italic *p?rātēr < PIE *bhre-H?ter-

Medially adjacent to *l, *r, or *u, *θ becomes b:

  • PItal. *wert?om "word" > *werθom > *wereom (? *werβom) > Lat. verbum (cf. English word < *wurda? < PIE *w?dhom, Lithuanian va?das "name")
  • PItal. *rut?ros "red" > *ruθros > *rueros (? *ruβros) > Latin ruber (via *rubers < *rubrs < *rubros), cf. rubra fem. rubrum neut.
  • PItal. *-t?lo-/*-t?lā- "tool suffix" > Latin -bulum, -bula: PIE *peH?-dhlo- "nourishment" > PItal. *pā-t?lo- > *pāθlo- > Latin pābulum; PIE *suH-dhleH?- "sewing implement" > PItal. *sūt?lā > *sūθlā > Latin sūbula "cobbler's awl"
Intervocalic Latin -b- is from PIE *bh, *s, and (rarely and problematically) *b: Lat. ambō "both" < PIE *ambh- or *H?embh- (cf. Greek amphi-); Lat. crābrō "hornet" < *??Hs-ron- (cf. Vedic ?īr?n- "hornet"); Lat. cannabis "hemp" (cf. Old English h?nep "hemp"). The change of *-sr- to -br- is itself presumably via *-θr- > *-er- > *-βr-.

Elsewhere, *θ becomes d:

  • PItal. *met?yo- "middle" > *meθyo- > Pre-Lat. *meeyo- > Lat. medius (three syllables; PIE *medhyo-, cf. Sanskrit madhya-, Greek més(s)os < *meth-yo-)
  • PItal. *p?eyt?- > *feyθ- > *feye- > Lat. fīdus "trusting" (cf. Greek peíthomai "am persuaded", English bid "order, ask")
Intervocalic -d- in Latin comes from PIE *d in ped- "foot", sīdere "to sit down", cord- "heart"

There is no alternation to give away the historical story, there, via internal reconstruction; the evidence for these changes is almost entirely from comparative reconstruction. That reconstruction makes it easy to unriddle the story behind the weird forms of the Latin paradigm jubeō "order", jussī perfect, jussus participle. If the root is inherited, it would have to have been PIE *yewdh-.

Unconditioned merger

edit

Unconditioned merger, that is, complete loss of a contrast between two or more phonemes, is not very common. Most mergers are conditioned. That is, most apparent mergers of A and B have an environment or two in which A did something else, such as drop or merge with C.[citation needed]

Typical is the unconditioned merger seen in the Celtic conflation of the PIE plain voiced series of stops with the breathy-voiced series: *bh, *dh, *?h, *gh are indistinguishable in Celtic etymology from the reflexes of *b *d *? *g. The collapse of the contrast cannot be stated in whole-series terms because the labiovelars do not co-operate. PIE *g? everywhere falls together with the reflexes of *b and *bh as Proto-Celtic *b, but *g?h seems to have become PCelt. *g?, lining up with PCelt. *k? < PIE *k?.

Examples

edit
  • OE y and y (short and long high front rounded vowels) fell together with i and í via a simple phonetic unrounding: OE hypp, cynn, cyssan, brycg, fyllan, fyr, mys, bryd became modern hip, kin, kiss, bridge, fill, fire, mice, bride. There is no way to tell by inspection whether a modern /i ay/ goes back to a rounded or an unrounded vowel. The change is not even reflected in modern spelling since it took place too early to be captured in Middle English Spelling conventions. Of course, current spellings like type, thyme, psyche, etc., have nothing to do with OE y = /y/.
  • There is a massive, consistent body of evidence that PIE *l and *r merged totally in Proto-Indo-Iranian, as did PIE *e *o *a into Proto-Indo-Iranian *a.
  • The evolution of Romance shows a systematic collection of unconditioned mergers in connection with the loss of Latin vowel length. Latin had ten vowels, five long and five short (i, ī; e, ē; a, ā; and so on). In the variety of Romance underlying Sardo and some other dialects of the islands, the ten vowels simply fell together pairwise: in no way are Latin e, ē, say, reflected differently. In Proto-Western-Romance, the ancestor of French, Iberian, Italian north of the Spezia-Rimini line, etc., however, things happened differently: Latin /a ā/ merged totally, as in Sardo, but the other vowels all behaved differently. Upon losing the feature of length, Latin /ī ū/ merged with nothing, but the short high vowels, front and back, merged with the long mid vowels: thus, Latin /i ē/ are uniformly reflected as PWRom. *? (in the standard Romance notation), and /u ō/ become *?. PWRom. *? is reflected in French (in open syllables) as /wa/ (spelled oi); voile "sail", foin "hay", doigt "finger", quoi "what", are from Latin vēlum, fēnum, digitus (via *dictu), quid, respectively. There is no way of telling in French which one of the two Latin vowels is the source of any given /wa/.

Another example is provided by Japonic languages. Old Japanese had 8 vowels; it has been reduced to 5 in modern Japanese.

Split

edit

In a split (Hoenigswald's "secondary split"), a new contrast arises when allophones of a phoneme cease being in complementary distribution and are therefore necessarily independent structure points, i.e. contrastive. This mostly comes about because of some loss of distinctiveness in the environment of one or more allophones of a phoneme.

A simple example is the rise of the contrast between nasal and oral vowels in French. A full account of this history is complicated by the subsequent changes in the phonetics of the nasal vowels, but the development can be compendiously illustrated via the present-day French phonemes /a/ and /?/:

  • Step 1: *a > *? when a nasal immediately follows: *?antu "song" > [t??ntu] (still phonemically /t?antu/);
  • Step 2: at some point in the history of French when speakers consistently stopped making an oral closure with the tongue, we had [t??t], that is /t??t/ (if not /??t/) and finally, with the loss of the final stop, modern French /??/ chant "song", distinct from French /?a/ chat "cat" solely by the contrast between the nasal and the oral articulation of the vowels, and thus with many other forms in which /a/ and /?/ contrast.
Note 1: the nasalization of a vowel before a nasal is found very widely in the world's languages, but is not at all universal. In modern French, for example, vowels before a nasal are oral. That they used to be nasalized, like the vowels before lost nasals, is indicated by certain phonetic changes not always reflected in the orthography: Fr. femme "woman" /fam/ (with the lowering of [??] (nasalized [?]) to *? prior to denasalization).
Note 2: unusually for a split, the history of the French innovation, even including some changes in vowel cavity features, can be readily inferred by internal reconstruction. This is because the contrastive feature [nasal] in a vowel system usually has a nasal in its history, which makes for straightforward surmises. There are also clear alternations, as /b??/ "good" (masc.) vs. /b?n/ (fem.), while such pairs as /fin/ "fine" (fem.) and /f??/ (masc.) together with derivatives like raffiné /rafine/ "refined" indicate what happened to nasalized *i.

Phonemic split was a major factor in the creation of the contrast between voiced and voiceless fricatives in English. Originally, to oversimplify a bit, Old English fricatives were voiced between voiced sounds and voiceless elsewhere. Thus /f/ was [f] in fisc [fi?] "fish", fyllen "to fill" [fyllen], h?ft "prisoner", oftyrsted [ofθyrsted] "athirst", líf "life", wulf "wolf". But in say the dative singular of "life", that is lífe, the form was [li:ve] (as in English alive, being an old prepositional phrase on lífe); the plural of wulf, wulfas, was [wulvas], as still seen in wolves. The voiced fricative is typically seen in verbs, too (often with variations in vowel length of diverse sources): gift but give, shelf but shelve. Such alternations are to be seen even in loan words, as proof vs prove (though not as a rule in borrowed plurals, thus proofs, uses, with voiceless fricatives).

Note 1: unlike the French example, there is no chance of recovering the historical source of the alternations in English between /s θ f/ and /z e v/ merely through inspection of the modern forms. The conditioning factor (original location of the voiced alternants between vowels, for example) is quite lost and with little reason to even suspect the original state of affairs; and anyway the original distributions have been much disturbed by analogical leveling. Worthy and (in some dialects) greasy have voiced fricative (next to the voiceless ones in worth and grease) but adjectives in -y otherwise do not alternate: bossy, glassy, leafy, earthy, breathy, saucy, etc (cf. glaze, leaves, breathe, and note that even in dialects with /z/ in greasy, the verb to grease always has /s/).
Note 2: the phoneme /?/ does not alternate with /?/ (and never did). In native words, /?/ is from *sk, and either the change of this sequence to /?/ postdated the rearranging of voicing in pre-Old English fricatives, or else it was phonetically long between vowels, originally, much like the /?/ of present-day Italian (pesce "fish" is phonetically [pe??e]) and long fricatives, just like sequences of fricatives, were always voiceless in Old English, as in cyssan "to kiss". The Early Modern English development of /?/ < */sj/, as in nation, mission, assure, vastly postdated the period when fricatives became voiced between vowels.
Note 3: a common misstatement of cases like OE /f/ > Modern English /f, v/ is that a "new phoneme" has been created. Not so. A new contrast has been created. Both NE /f/ and /v/ are new phonemes, differing in phonetic specifications and distribution from OE /f/. Without doubt, one component in this misunderstanding is the orthography. If, instead of speaking of the development of Old English /f/ we said that OE /?/ split into /f/ and /v/, there would presumably be less confused talk of "a" new phoneme arising in the process.

Loss

edit

In Hoenigswald's original scheme, loss, the disappearance of a segment, or even of a whole phoneme, was treated as a form of merger, depending on whether the loss was conditioned or unconditioned. The "element" that a vanished segment or phoneme merged with was "zero".

The situation in which a highly inflected language has formations without any affix at all (Latin alter "(the) other", for example) is quite common, but it is the only one (nominative singular masculine: altera nominative singular feminine, alterum accusative singular masculine, etc.) of the 30 forms that make up the paradigm that is not explicitly marked with endings for gender, number, and case.

From a historical perspective, there is no problem since alter is from *alteros (overtly nominative singular and masculine), with the regular loss of the short vowel after *-r- and the truncation of the resulting word-final cluster *-rs. Descriptively, however, it is problematic to say that the "nominative singular masculine" is signaled by the absence of any affix. It is simpler to view alter as more than what it looks like, /alter?/, "marked" for case, number, and gender by an affix, like the other 29 forms in the paradigm. It is merely that the "marker" in question is not a phoneme or sequence of phonemes but the element /?/.

Along the way, it is hard to know when to stop positing zeros and whether to regard one zero as different from another. For example, if the zero not-marking can (as in he can) as "third person singular" is the same zero that not-marks deer as "plural", or if are both basically a single morphological placeholder. If it is determined that there is a zero on the end of deer in three deer, it is uncertain whether English adjectives agree with the number of the noun they modify, using the same zero affix. (Deictics do so: compare this deer, these deer.) In some theories of syntax it is useful to have an overt marker on a singular noun in a sentence such as My head hurts because the syntactic mechanism needs something explicit to generate the singular suffix on the verb. Thus, all English singular nouns may be marked with yet another zero.

It seems possible to avoid all those issues by considering loss as a separate basic category of phonological change, and leave zero out of it.

As stated above, one can regard loss as both a kind of conditioned merger (when only some expressions of a phoneme are lost) and a disappearance of a whole structure point. The former is much more common than the latter.

  • In Latin are many consonant clusters that lose a member or two such as these: tostus "toasted, dried" < *torstos, multrum "milking stool" < *molktrom, scultus "carved" < *scolptos, cēna "dinner" < *kertsnā, lūna "moon" < *louwksnā ("lantern" or the like).
  • Greek lost all stops from the end of a word (so *k?it "what" > Greek ti, *de??t "ten" > déka, *wanakt "O prince" > ána), but stops generally survive elsewhere. PIE *s drops medially between voiced sounds in Greek but is preserved in final position and in some consonant clusters.
  • Old English [x] (voiceless velar fricative) is everywhere lost as such, but usually leaves traces behind (transphonologization). In furh "furrow" and mearh "marrow", it vocalizes. It is elided (with varying effects on the preceding vowel, such as lengthening) in night, knight, might, taught, naught, freight, fought, plow (Brit. plough, OE plōh), bought, through, though, slaughter; but /f/ in laugh, trough, tough, enough (and daughter can be found in The Pilgrim's Progress rhyming with after, and the spelling dafter is actually attested) The /x/ phoneme still exists in some onomatopoeiac words, like "ugh" (note the spelling uses gh, which indicates that when they were coined, there was still some understanding of the phonemic meaning of gh), "yech" and "chutzpah".
  • /g k/ are lost in English in word-initial position before /n/: gnaw, gnat, knight, know. /t/ is lost after fricatives before nasals and /l/: soften, castle, bristle, chestnut, Christmas, hasten
  • In many words, /f/ (that is, Old English [v]) was lost between vowels: auger, hawk, newt < OE nafogar, hafoc, efete ("lizard"), and in some alternative (poetic) forms: e'en "evening", o'er "over", e'er "ever"; Scottish siller "silver", and others.

The ends of words often have sound laws that apply there only, and many such special developments consist of the loss of a segment. The early history and prehistory of English has seen several waves of loss of elements, vowels and consonants alike, from the ends of words, first in Proto-Germanic, then to Proto-West-Germanic, then to Old and Middle and Modern English, shedding bits from the ends of words at every step of the way. There is in Modern English next to nothing left of the elaborate inflectional and derivational apparatus of PIE or of Proto-Germanic because of the successive ablation of the phonemes making up these suffixes.

Total unconditional loss is, as mentioned, not very common. Latin /h/ appears to have been lost everywhere in all varieties of Proto-Romance except Romanian. Proto-Indo-European laryngeals survived as consonants only in Anatolian languages but left plenty of traces of their former presence (see laryngeal theory).

Phonemic differentiation

edit

Phonemic differentiation is the phenomenon of a language maximizing the acoustic distance between its phonemes.

Examples

edit

For example, in many languages, including English, most front vowels are unrounded, while most back vowels are rounded. There are no languages in which all front vowels are rounded and all back vowels are unrounded. The most likely explanation for this[citation needed] is that front vowels have a higher second formant (F2) than back vowels, and unrounded vowels have a higher F2 than rounded vowels. Thus unrounded front vowels and rounded back vowels have maximally different F2s, enhancing their phonemic differentiation.

Phonemic differentiation can have an effect on diachronic sound change. In chain shifts, phonemic differentiation is maintained, while in phonemic mergers it is lost. Phonemic splits involve the creation of two phonemes out of one, which then tend to diverge because of phonemic differentiation.

Chain shifts

edit

In a chain shift, one phoneme moves in acoustic space, causing other phonemes to move as well to maintain optimal phonemic differentiation. An example from American English is the Northern cities vowel shift [1], where the raising of /?/ has triggered a fronting of /ɑ/, which in turn has triggered a lowering of /?/, and so forth.

Phonemic mergers

edit

If a phoneme moves in acoustic space, but its neighbors do not move in a chain shift, a phonemic merger may occur. In that case, a single phoneme results where an earlier stage of the language had two phonemes (that is also called phonetic neutralization). A well known example of a phonemic merger in American English is the cot–caught merger by which the vowel phonemes /ɑ/ and /?/ (illustrated by the words cot and caught respectively) have merged into a single phoneme in some accents.

Phonemic splits

edit

In a phonemic split, a phoneme at an earlier stage of the language is divided into two phonemes over time. Usually, it happens when a phoneme has two allophones appearing in different environments, but sound change eliminates the distinction between the two environments. For example, in umlaut in the Germanic languages, the back vowels /u, o/ originally had front rounded allophones [y, ?] before the vowel /i/ in a following syllable. When sound change caused the syllables containing /i/ to be lost, a phonemic split resulted, making /y, ?/ distinct phonemes.

It is sometimes difficult to determine whether a split or a merger has happened if one dialect has two phonemes corresponding to a single phoneme in another dialect; diachronic research is usually required to determine the dialect that is conservative and the one that is innovative.

When phonemic change occurs differently in the standard language and in dialects, the dialect pronunciation is considered nonstandard and may be stigmatized. In descriptive linguistics, however, the question of which splits and mergers are prestigious and which are stigmatized is irrelevant. However, such stigmatization can lead to hypercorrection, when the dialect speakers attempt to imitate the standard language but overshoot, as with the footstrut split, where failing to make the split is stigmatized in Northern England, and speakers of non-splitting accents often try to introduce it into their speech, sometimes resulting in hypercorrections such as pronouncing pudding /p?d??/.

Occasionally, speakers of one accent may believe the speakers of another accent to have undergone a merger, when there has really been a chain shift.

See also

edit

References

edit

Notes

edit
  1. ^ a b Henrich Hock, Hans (1991). Principles of Historical Linguistics (Second ed.). Berlin; New York: Walter de Gruyter. pp. 53–4.
  2. ^ a b Crowley, Terry; Bowern, Claire (2010). An Introduction to Historical Linguistics (4th ed.). Oxford; New York: Oxford University Press. p. 69. ISBN 9780195365542.
  3. ^ The same PIE root *H?eg?nós yielded Greek ?μν?? ámnos "lamb". In Latin, PIE labiovelars were regularly delabialized before another consonant: relictus "left behind" < *lik?-to- (cf. relinquō "leave behind", Greek leipō).
  4. ^ While Roman grammarians generally make some fairly fine observations about Latin phonetics, they do not mention g = [?] despite being thoroughly familiar with the idea from Greek orthography, where |γ| = [?] before /k/ and /g/, as in agkúlos "bent" /a?kylos/, ággellos "messenger" /á?ɡellos/. This is likely to be a mere oversight.

Sources

edit
  1. Hale, M. (2007), Historical linguistics: Theory and method, Oxford, Blackwell[2][3]
  2. Hale, M., Kissock, M., & Reiss, C. (2014) An I-Language Approach to Phonologization and Lexification. Chapter 20. The Oxford Handbook of Historical Phonology. Edited by Patrick Honeybone and Joseph Salmons
  3. Hoenigswald, H. (1965). Language change and linguistic reconstruction. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
甙是什么意思 第一次坐飞机要注意什么 焦急的什么 糖尿病人适合喝什么茶 牛皮癣是什么
坐北朝南是什么意思 什么可以消肿快的方法 阿迪达斯和三叶草有什么区别 反胃恶心吃什么药 梦见来月经是什么意思
女性肠痉挛有什么症状 中央电视台台长什么级别 多囊卵巢是什么原因造成的 代价是什么意思 低蛋白血症是什么意思
叶什么什么龙 轻浮是什么意思 闺房是什么意思 手心脚心热吃什么药 皮肤自愈能力差缺什么
开通花呗有什么风险hcv9jop4ns8r.cn 司令员是什么军衔hcv8jop3ns2r.cn 鱼豆腐是用什么做的hcv7jop9ns9r.cn 吃软不吃硬是什么生肖chuanglingweilai.com 宋五行属什么hcv7jop6ns6r.cn
丁香花长什么样hcv9jop2ns8r.cn 为什么会梦到蛇520myf.com manu是什么意思youbangsi.com faleda是什么牌子的手表hcv9jop6ns9r.cn 势均力敌是什么意思hcv7jop5ns1r.cn
blk是什么意思aiwuzhiyu.com 马铃薯什么时候传入中国hcv9jop1ns1r.cn 花痴是什么意思hcv9jop1ns5r.cn 游园惊梦讲的是什么hcv7jop9ns7r.cn 仲什么意思hcv9jop4ns6r.cn
鹅蛋什么人不能吃hcv8jop0ns3r.cn 2b铅笔和hb铅笔有什么区别hcv9jop3ns4r.cn 三七和田七有什么区别hcv8jop6ns9r.cn 左下腹痛挂什么科hcv9jop2ns5r.cn 血小板高是什么引起的hcv7jop9ns2r.cn
百度